Infanticide in Primates: Difference between revisions
(41 intermediate revisions by the same user not shown) | |||
Line 1: | Line 1: | ||
==Introduction== | ==Introduction== | ||
<b> Infanticide (in animals) </b> generally refers to the killing of an infant or a young offspring by an adult or mature individual of the same species and is observed in a variety of species ranging from humans to microscopic rotifers and especially in primates. Both males and females can be the perpetrators of infanticide in animals and both parents (filial infanticide) and non-parent individuals have been observed to display the behavior. Filial infanticide, which can be accompanied by cannibalism (filial cannibalism), is widespread in fishes and is also seen in terrestrial animals. It has been estimated that infanticide occurs in 25% of all mammals and, in some of these populations, infanticide is a major contributor to infant mortality.<ref name=Reuters>[Dunham, Will. “Infanticide Common among Adult Males in Many Mammal Species.” Reuters, Thomson Reuters, 13 Nov. 2014, https://www.reuters.com/article/us-science-infanticide/infanticide-common-among-adult-males-in-many-mammal-species-idUSKCN0IX2BA20141113.] </ref><br> | <b> Infanticide (in animals) </b> generally refers to the killing of an infant or a young offspring by an adult or mature individual of the same species and is observed in a variety of species ranging from humans to microscopic rotifers and especially in primates. Both males and females can be the perpetrators of infanticide in animals and both parents (filial infanticide) and non-parent individuals have been observed to display the behavior.<ref name=MAT1 /><ref name=agonistic /><ref name=CHIMPS1 /> Filial infanticide, which can be accompanied by cannibalism (filial cannibalism), is widespread in fishes and is also seen in terrestrial animals. It has been estimated that infanticide occurs in 25% of all mammals and, in some of these populations, infanticide is a major contributor to infant mortality.<ref name=Reuters>[Dunham, Will. “Infanticide Common among Adult Males in Many Mammal Species.” Reuters, Thomson Reuters, 13 Nov. 2014, https://www.reuters.com/article/us-science-infanticide/infanticide-common-among-adult-males-in-many-mammal-species-idUSKCN0IX2BA20141113.] </ref><br> | ||
[[Image:Infanticide_1.jpg|thumb|400px|right| An adult baboon looking at a dependent young<br> https://www.upi.com/Science_News/2014/11/13/Big-testes-are-sign-that-a-species-has-history-of-infanticide/8601415916184/.]] | |||
<br> | <br> | ||
Many primates such as the gorilla, chimpanzee, baboon, and langur have been known to practice infanticide while others, such as the orangutan, bonobo and mouse lemur have not been observed to do so.<ref name=Reuters /> Although previously considered pathological and maladaptive and attributed to environmental conditions such as overcrowding and captivity <ref name=pops11>[http://pages.ucsd.edu/~jmoore/publications/DensityInfanticide.html Moore, Jim. “Population Density, Social Pathology, and Behavioral Ecology.” Primates, vol. 40, no. 1, 1999, pp. 1–22., doi:10.1007/bf02557698.]</ref>, there are currently several explanations for why infanticide has evolved in non-human primate communities. Interestingly, the frequency and distribution of infanticide seems to suggest that there is a correlation between infanticide risk and social organization in mammals and primates. <ref name=MAT2 /><ref name=evo1 /> Male infanticide occurs most frequently in social species, less frequently in solitary species and least frequently in monogamous species.<ref name=evo1>[https://science.sciencemag.org/content/346/6211/841 Lukas, D., and E. Huchard. “The Evolution of Infanticide by Males in Mammalian Societies.” Science, vol. 346, no. 6211, 2014, pp. 841–844., doi:10.1126/science.1257226.]</ref> The fitness advantage the behavior has, the associated costs and the different ways it is thought to have affected the structure of primate populations make the evolution and genetics of the behavior very interesting to study. | |||
<br> | <br> | ||
==Section 1 Evolution and Genetics of Behavior== | ==Section 1 Evolution and Genetics of the Behavior== | ||
Several explanations have been proposed for the existence and evolution of infanticide in non- human primates. The benefits of infanticide for male non-human primates, and its costs to females, probably vary across mammalian social and mating systems <ref name=evo1 /> and between different primate species as well. Infanticidal behavior can be adaptive because of nutritive benefits individuals gain from cannibalism, conservation of resources by eliminating competitors and paternal manipulation in which parents terminate investment in particular offspring by killing (and often consuming) them <ref name=IFS>[https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4448612/ Palombit, Ryne A. “Infanticide as Sexual Conflict: Coevolution of Male Strategies and Female Counterstrategies.” Cold Spring Harbor Perspectives in Biology, vol. 7, no. 6, 2015, doi:10.1101/cshperspect.a017640]</ref>. The prevailing adaptive explanation for the behavior, however, has been the sexual-selection hypothesis according to which males increase reproductive opportunity by killing unrelated, unweaned offspring, thus hastening the mother's next ovulation, at which time the infanticidal male can mate with her <ref name=hrdy>[https://www.researchgate.net/publication/222438680_Infanticide_among_Animals_A_Review_Classification_and_Examination_of_the_Implications_for_the_Reproductive_Strategies_of_Females Hrdy, Sarah Blaffer. “Infanticide among Animals: A Review, Classification, and Examination of the Implications for the Reproductive Strategies of Females.” Ethology and Sociobiology, vol. 1, no. 1, 1979, pp. 13–40., doi:10.1016/0162-3095(79)90004-9]</ref>. Alternate nonadaptive explanations such as social-pathology and side effect hypotheses have also been proposed.<ref name=hrdy /><br> | Several explanations have been proposed for the existence and evolution of infanticide in non- human primates. The benefits of infanticide for male non-human primates, and its costs to females, probably vary across mammalian social and mating systems <ref name=evo1 /> and between different primate species as well. Infanticidal behavior can be adaptive because of nutritive benefits individuals gain from cannibalism, conservation of resources by eliminating competitors and paternal manipulation in which parents terminate investment in particular offspring by killing (and often consuming) them <ref name=IFS>[https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4448612/ Palombit, Ryne A. “Infanticide as Sexual Conflict: Coevolution of Male Strategies and Female Counterstrategies.” Cold Spring Harbor Perspectives in Biology, vol. 7, no. 6, 2015, doi:10.1101/cshperspect.a017640]</ref>. The prevailing adaptive explanation for the behavior, however, has been the sexual-selection hypothesis according to which males increase reproductive opportunity by killing unrelated, unweaned offspring, thus hastening the mother's next ovulation, at which time the infanticidal male can mate with her <ref name=hrdy>[https://www.researchgate.net/publication/222438680_Infanticide_among_Animals_A_Review_Classification_and_Examination_of_the_Implications_for_the_Reproductive_Strategies_of_Females Hrdy, Sarah Blaffer. “Infanticide among Animals: A Review, Classification, and Examination of the Implications for the Reproductive Strategies of Females.” Ethology and Sociobiology, vol. 1, no. 1, 1979, pp. 13–40., doi:10.1016/0162-3095(79)90004-9]</ref>. Alternate nonadaptive explanations such as social-pathology and side effect hypotheses have also been proposed.<ref name=hrdy /><br> | ||
Males in primate groups often exploit infants to which they are not related in ways that lead to the death of the infant.<ref name=IFS /> Several primate species have been observed to engage in the killing and subsequent cannibalism of infants.<ref name=IFS /> This type of behavior has also been seen in the great apes such as bonobos and chimpanzees.<ref> Callaway, Ewen. “Hippy Apes Caught Cannibalising Their Young.” New Scientist, 1 Feb. 2010, https://www.newscientist.com/article/dn18454-hippy-apes-caught-cannibalising-their-young/</ref><ref name=CHIMPS1>[https://link.springer.com/article/10.1007/BF02382746 Hamai, Miya, et al. “New Records of within-Group Infanticide and Cannibalism in Wild Chimpanzees.” Primates, vol. 33, no. 2, 1992, pp. 151–162., doi:10.1007/bf02382746.]</ref> In chimpanzees, cannibalism has been suggested to be the primary function of infanticide.<ref name=CHIMPS1 /> Infants may also be used as protective buffers in primate groups during | Males in primate groups often exploit infants to which they are not related in ways that lead to the death of the infant.<ref name=IFS /> Several primate species have been observed to engage in the killing and subsequent cannibalism of infants.<ref name=IFS /> This type of behavior has also been seen in the great apes such as bonobos and chimpanzees.<ref name=hip> Callaway, Ewen. “Hippy Apes Caught Cannibalising Their Young.” New Scientist, 1 Feb. 2010, https://www.newscientist.com/article/dn18454-hippy-apes-caught-cannibalising-their-young/</ref><ref name=CHIMPS1>[https://link.springer.com/article/10.1007/BF02382746 Hamai, Miya, et al. “New Records of within-Group Infanticide and Cannibalism in Wild Chimpanzees.” Primates, vol. 33, no. 2, 1992, pp. 151–162., doi:10.1007/bf02382746.]</ref> In chimpanzees, cannibalism has been suggested to be the primary function of infanticide.<ref name=CHIMPS1 /> Infants may also be used as protective buffers in primate groups during agonistic encounters (conflicts) between males which can result in the death of the infant.<ref name=agonistic>[https://link.springer.com/article/10.1007/BF02382293 Dunbar, R. I. M. “Infant-Use by Male Gelada in Agonistic Contexts: Agonistic Buffering, Progeny Protection or Soliciting Support?” Primates, vol. 25, no. 1, 1984, pp. 28–35., doi:10.1007/bf02382293]</ref> Females in primate groups also kill or cannibalize infants although usually for motivations different from nutrition.<ref name=MAT1>[https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3068257/ Culot, Laurence, et al. “Reproductive Failure, Possible Maternal Infanticide, and Cannibalism in Wild Moustached Tamarins, Saguinus Mystax.” Primates, vol. 52, no. 2, 2011, pp. 179–186., doi:10.1007/s10329-011-0238-6. ]</ref><br> | ||
Maternal infanticide, although very rarely observed in non-human primates, has been considered as a way of paternal manipulation to end investment in certain offspring. <ref name=MAT1 /> A study investigating maternal infanticide in tamarins has concluded that maternal infanticide can occur in a group with a poor capacity to raise offspring, multiple breeding females and estimated low survival probability for the infants that are killed because of injuries or apparent weakness.<ref name=MAT1 /> The same study also found that the infants that were killed were cannibalized by their mothers. However, the study indicates that the mothers likely did not kill their offspring to exploit the meat.<ref name=MAT1 /> The victim of infanticide for parental manipulation does not necessarily have to be defective but can simply be born at a bad time, consequently being a supplementary cost to the parents. <ref name=hrdy /><br> | Maternal infanticide, although very rarely observed in non-human primates, has been considered as a way of paternal manipulation to end investment in certain offspring. <ref name=MAT1 /> A study investigating maternal infanticide in tamarins has concluded that maternal infanticide can occur in a group with a poor capacity to raise offspring, multiple breeding females and estimated low survival probability for the infants that are killed because of injuries or apparent weakness.<ref name=MAT1 /> The same study also found that the infants that were killed were cannibalized by their mothers. However, the study indicates that the mothers likely did not kill their offspring to exploit the meat.<ref name=MAT1 /> The victim of infanticide for parental manipulation does not necessarily have to be defective but can simply be born at a bad time, consequently being a supplementary cost to the parents. <ref name=hrdy /><br> | ||
Line 19: | Line 19: | ||
On the other hand, the evolution of (nonparental) male infanticide has often been explained by the sexual selection hypothesis, which posits that infanticide improves male reproductive success by shortening the interbirth intervals of the mothers of the killed offspring.<ref name=hrdy /><ref name=Se2>[https://link.springer.com/article/10.1007/s10682-017-9925-0 Harano, Tomohiro, and Nobuyuki Kutsukake. “The Evolution of Male Infanticide in Relation to Sexual Selection in Mammalian Carnivores.” Evolutionary Ecology, vol. 32, no. 1, 2017, pp. 1–8., doi:10.1007/s10682-017-9925-0]</ref> Notably in primates, male infanticide can increase the reproductive success of male individuals by accelerating the return of females to fertilizable condition and ensuring that a greater percentages of the individuals’ offspring become the next generation when the male kills the offspring of other rival males <ref name=IFS />. The sexual selection hypothesis is supported by findings that indicate that infanticide mostly occurs in social species, less in solitary species, and least in monogamous species since according to the sexual selection hypothesis, infanticide would be most adaptive in stable bisexual groups where a few males monopolize reproduction over short periods of time <ref name=evo1 />.In social species where a few males monopolize reproduction over short periods, killing the offspring of males who had previously been monopolizing reproduction would be adaptive for the individuals committing infanticide. In monogamous species, since pairs of reproducing adults live together with their offspring their whole lives, male infanticide of unrelated infants would not be as adaptive for individuals. A study into infanticide in langur monkeys has also shown that male attackers were not related to their infant victim and were likely the fathers of subsequent infants based on DNA analyses, further supporting the hypothesis.<ref name=lan1>[https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC1689922/ Borries, Carola, et al. “DNA Analyses Support the Hypothesis That Infanticide Is Adaptive in Langur Monkeys.” Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, vol. 266, no. 1422, July 1999, pp. 901–904., doi:10.1098/rspb.1999.0721.]</ref><br> | On the other hand, the evolution of (nonparental) male infanticide has often been explained by the sexual selection hypothesis, which posits that infanticide improves male reproductive success by shortening the interbirth intervals of the mothers of the killed offspring.<ref name=hrdy /><ref name=Se2>[https://link.springer.com/article/10.1007/s10682-017-9925-0 Harano, Tomohiro, and Nobuyuki Kutsukake. “The Evolution of Male Infanticide in Relation to Sexual Selection in Mammalian Carnivores.” Evolutionary Ecology, vol. 32, no. 1, 2017, pp. 1–8., doi:10.1007/s10682-017-9925-0]</ref> Notably in primates, male infanticide can increase the reproductive success of male individuals by accelerating the return of females to fertilizable condition and ensuring that a greater percentages of the individuals’ offspring become the next generation when the male kills the offspring of other rival males <ref name=IFS />. The sexual selection hypothesis is supported by findings that indicate that infanticide mostly occurs in social species, less in solitary species, and least in monogamous species since according to the sexual selection hypothesis, infanticide would be most adaptive in stable bisexual groups where a few males monopolize reproduction over short periods of time <ref name=evo1 />.In social species where a few males monopolize reproduction over short periods, killing the offspring of males who had previously been monopolizing reproduction would be adaptive for the individuals committing infanticide. In monogamous species, since pairs of reproducing adults live together with their offspring their whole lives, male infanticide of unrelated infants would not be as adaptive for individuals. A study into infanticide in langur monkeys has also shown that male attackers were not related to their infant victim and were likely the fathers of subsequent infants based on DNA analyses, further supporting the hypothesis.<ref name=lan1>[https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC1689922/ Borries, Carola, et al. “DNA Analyses Support the Hypothesis That Infanticide Is Adaptive in Langur Monkeys.” Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, vol. 266, no. 1422, July 1999, pp. 901–904., doi:10.1098/rspb.1999.0721.]</ref><br> | ||
==Section 2 | While the majority of the explanations for why infanticide exists as a behavior posit that infanticide is an adaptive heritable behavior in primates, some have explained infanticide as a pathological behavior that is non-adaptive and does not need an evolutionary explanation.<ref name=IFS /><ref name=pops11 /> One such hypothesis, social pathology, argues that infanticide is genetically inconsequential and maladaptive in normal conditions.<ref name=IFS /> The hypothesis states that infanticide results from certain conditions created by human-induced disturbances or changes in nature.<ref name=IFS /> Social disturbances such as overcrowding have been documented to have resulted in infanticide in rats <ref>[https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2636191/ Ramsden, Edmund. “The Urban Aminal: Population Density and Social Pathology in Rodent and Humans.” Bulletin of the World Health Organization, vol. 87, no. 2, Jan. 2009, pp. 82–82., doi:10.2471/blt.09.062836]</ref>, and similar factors may be at play in primate communities in which infanticide has been observed. Though the social pathology hypothesis may apply to some cases of infanticide <ref>[https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0376635708000053 Feh, C., and B. Munkhtuya. “Male Infanticide and Paternity Analyses in a Socially Natural Herd of Przewalskis Horses: Sexual Selection?” Behavioral Processes, vol. 78, no. 3, 2008, pp. 335–339., doi:10.1016/j.beproc.2007.12.009. ]</ref>, increasing evidence has pointed to the sexual selection hypothesis for the most likely explanation of the prevalence of infanticide in primate communities.<ref name=just12>[https://www.cambridge.org/core/books/infanticide-by-males-and-its-implications/behavioral-ecology-of-infanticide-by-males/F4D6C5A70471B89862BEE763C3C5C81B Janson, Charles H., and Carel P. Van Schaik. “The Behavioral Ecology of Infanticide by Males.” Infanticide by Males and Its Implications, Feb. 2000, pp. 469–494., doi:10.1017/cbo9780511542312.021.]</ref><br> | ||
[[Image:Infant_2.jpg|thumb|400px|right| Male infanticide has been reported for about half of all species this sample (open circles) and seems to have evolved independently multiple times. It most frequently occurs in social species (dark gray branches), less frequently in solitary species (light gray branches), and least frequently in monogamous species (black branches). Animal drawings are from phylopic.org. <br> https://science.sciencemag.org/content/346/6211/841]] | |||
One such evidence supporting the sexual selection hypothesis comes from a study conducted on house mice showing that infanticide is a highly liable and heritable behavior.<ref name=rats1><[https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/8129694 Perrigo, Glenn, et al. “Genetic Mediation of Infanticide and Parental Behavior in Male and Female Domestic and Wild Stock House Mice.” Behavior Genetics, vol. 23, no. 6, 1993, pp. 525–531., doi:10.1007/bf01068143]</ref> The study also concludes that there is a sex difference in the genetic substrate that regulates the inheritance of infanticidal behavior.<ref name=rats1 /> Recent advances in technology have shown that genetics heavily influences behavior or behavioral phenotype in primates and have led to the beginning of the identification of genes mediating different behaviors <ref name=eco2>[https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/29380886 Rogers, Jeffrey. “The Behavioral Genetics of Nonhuman Primates: Status and Prospects.” American Journal of Physical Anthropology, vol. 165, 2018, pp. 23–36., doi:10.1002/ajpa.23384.]</ref>. Though it is not clear which genes are involved in primate infanticide, there is likely a genetic basis for the behavior that is yet to be discovered.<br> | |||
Variation in social organization and mating systems may have favored or prevented the evolution of infanticide by males in mammalian species. The distribution of male infanticide is closely associated with the absence of seasonal or annual breeding. Seasonal breeders cannot resume (reproductive) cycling before the next breeding season. <ref name=evo1 /> As a result, infanticide in seasonal breeders doesn't accelerate the mother's return to sexual activity, relatively decreasing its adaptive advantage.<ref name=evo1 /> Furthermore, infanticide is more likely to occur in mammalian species where there are more males per females in social groups and dominant males obtain a higher share of reproduction in a given season but maintain their dominant position for short periods of time.<ref name=evo1 /> In these species, new dominant males could kill the infants of the previous dominant male so that more of the new dominant male’s genes are transferred to the next generation.<ref name=evo1 /> | |||
==Section 2 Costs of Infanticide and Counter-startegies== | |||
Whatever the benefit of male infanticide to the perpetrators, there is little doubt that it usually imposes a substantive cost on females’ fitness and reproductive success (as well as infants), particularly in the slowly reproducing species in which infanticide due to sexual selection is expected to operate.<ref name=IFS /> This is thought to have led to the evolution of several female counter-strategies against infanticide ranging from physical defense of infants from infanticidal individuals to promiscuous mating to confuse paternity.<ref name=IFS /> In addition to these counter-strategies, males are also thought to display behavior that prevents infanticide when their own offspring is involved.<ref name=counter>[https://www.jstor.org/stable/3546678 Agrell, Jep, et al. “Counter-Strategies to Infanticide in Mammals: Costs and Consequences.” Oikos, vol. 83, no. 3, 1998, p. 507., doi:10.2307/3546678.]</ref><br> | |||
In many primate species, mothers have been documented to increase visual monitoring of potentially infanticidal male and/or offspring, and restrain and/or maintain proximity to infants more under conditions of heightened infanticide risk.<ref name=IFS /> Males may also physically defend their own offspring.<ref name=counter /> In social primates such as baboons and macaques, the main role of male-male aggression may be to provide exclusive reproductive access to females and defend them and their young from stranger males.<ref name=counter /> Both of these counter-strategies result in costs such as expenditure of extra energy and increased risk of injuries. <ref name=counter /> <br> | |||
Another female counter-strategy against male infanticide (carried out due to reasons outlined by the sexual selection hypothesis) that has thought to have evolved in primates is promiscuity or multi-male mating.<ref name=IFS /> Promiscuous mating as a result of some reproductive adaptations/changes creates paternity confusion among the primate group thereby decreasing the likelihood of infanticide since perpetrators would not be sure if they were killing their children.<ref name=counter /> Among primates, 62% of species with female susceptibility to infanticide show promiscuity, compared to the 11% of non-vulnerable species showing promiscuity. <ref>[https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511542312.016 Noordwijk, Maria A. Van, and Carel P. Van Schaik. “Reproductive Patterns in Eutherian Mammals: Adaptations against Infanticide?” Infanticide by Males and Its Implications, Feb. 2000, pp. 322–360., doi:10.1017/cbo9780511542312.016]</ref> This suggests that there is some correlation between infanticide and promiscuity in primates. Females in some primate species such as langur and gelada have also been known to terminate pregnancies to end investment in that offspring early after exposure to new replacement males through a process known as the Bruce effect.<ref name=IFS /> Other female reproductive counter-strategies include but are not limited to concealed ovulation (paternity is obscured by the absence of reliable signals of ovulation and/or by variability in the timing of ovulation) and copulation with specific males (like those most likely to commit infanticide or more likely to protect the offspring). <ref name=IFS /> A large set of potential female counterstrategies involves deterrence through cooperation with conspecifics (individuals of the same species). <ref name=IFS /> Females in some primate species may form associations or coalitions with other females and with males to defend their offspring and reduce the risk of infanticide. <ref name=IFS /><br> | |||
Both infanticide and the counter-strategies against can have their own costs. Increased risk of injuries for both the perpetrator and the defender is one cost. <ref name=counter /> Some of the counter strategies listed above such as multi-male mating or physical defense are generally associated with the increased expenditure of energy, increased exposure to predation and, obviously, increased risk of injury for both the perpetrator and defender.<ref name=counter /> Counter strategies like multi-male mating and female mate choice could limit choice for good genes since the females would be forced to mate with specific males, <ref name=counter /> possibly affecting populations as a whole. | |||
==Section 3 Effects on Social Organization of Primates== | |||
[[Image:Infant_3.jpg|thumb|300px|left| In some lineages in which males commit infanticide, testes appear to increase in size (relative to body mass) and after some time, male infanticide is lost when testes have become relatively large.<br> https://science.sciencemag.org/content/346/6211/841]] | |||
== | Male infanticide has been proposed to alter social evolution in mammals (and primates) by favoring female gregariousness or permanent male-female associations or by promoting a higher number of resident males that may help mothers to protect their infants from male intruders.<ref name=evo1 /> It has also been proposed that male infanticide likely led to social monogamy in primates and, potentially, humans.<ref name=MAT2>[https://www.pnas.org/content/110/33/13328 Opie, C., et al. “Male Infanticide Leads to Social Monogamy in Primates.” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, vol. 110, no. 33, 2013, pp. 13328–13332., doi:10.1073/pnas.1307903110]</ref> However, these theories have been criticized<ref name=evo1 /><ref name=MAT3>[https://www.pnas.org/content/110/33/13328 Dixson, A. F. “Male Infanticide and Primate Monogamy.” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, vol. 110, no. 51, May 2013, doi:10.1073/pnas.1318645110.]</ref> and some have pointed out that these theories are not based on enough data, but make assumptions that are likely incorrect.<ref name=MAT3 /> It is also important to consider that infanticide risk is low in solitary species, the ancestral mammalian social organization, and is unlikely to motivate evolutionary transitions from a solitary lifestyle to other social organizations.<ref name=evo1 /> Phylogenetic reconstructions have also shown that male infanticide is significantly more likely to evolve after transitions toward group living than in their solitary ancestors. <ref name=evo1 /> As mentioned above, male infanticide could have led to the evolution of multi-male mating or promiscuity of females in some primates species. <ref name=evo1 /><ref name=IFS /> In support of this hypothesis, phylogenetic reconstructions indicate that relative testis size (which is an indicator of sperm competition) is larger in lineages in which more time has elapsed since the evolution of infanticide. <ref name=evo1 /> In addition, infanticide is lost in lineages with large testes, indicating that infanticide may disappear where female attempts to prevent male sexual monopolization are successful. <ref name=evo1 /><br> | ||
==Section 4 Microbiome== | |||
According to the resource competition hypothesis animals engage in infanticide to exploit the infants in different ways. <ref name=IFS /> Adults of primate population often cannibalize the infant they kill, thereby gaining nutritive benefits from the action. <ref name=CHIMPS1 /><ref name=hip /> Since cannibalism can increase the risk of pathogen and disease transmission in animals <ref name=just20>[https://link.springer.com/article/10.1007/s10329-019-00747-8 Trapanese, Cinzia, et al. “Prolonged Care and Cannibalism of Infant Corpse by Relatives in Semi-Free-Ranging Capuchin Monkeys.” Primates, Mar. 2019, doi:10.1007/s10329-019-00747-8.]</ref>, infanticide followed by cannibalism may decrease the fitness of individuals in primate species engaging in this behavior. Although cannibalism may be an unlikely disease transmission pathway in many primate species because the epidemiological conditions necessary for its spread are rarely met in natural populations, it is suspected that such transmission occurs in animals such as chimpanzees where cannibalism occurs by socially organized groups of males (group cannibalism). <ref name=just21>[https://royalsocietypublishing.org/doi/pdf/10.1098/rspb.2006.0449 Rudolf, Volker H.w, and Janis Antonovics. “Disease Transmission by Cannibalism: Rare Event or Common Occurrence?” Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, vol. 274, no. 1614, 2007, pp. 1205–1210., doi:10.1098/rspb.2006.0449]</ref> Therefore, it is thought that group cannibalism including such cannibalism that follows infanticide (which has been documented in chimpanzees)<ref name=CHIMPS1 /><ref name=hip />, may play an important role in the transmission and maintenance of blood-borne infections like simian immunodeficiency virus or different types of hepatitis in these populations. <ref name=just21 /> | |||
==Conclusion== | |||
The adaptive significance of infanticide, if the behavior is considered adaptive for a population, most likely differs from population to population meaning the advantage of the strategy is multifarious, not unitary. <ref name=IFS /> It is important to consider various factors that could be affecting the frequency and distribution of infanticide (such as overcrowding in a population<ref name=IFS />) or those that could be affected by it (such as testes size<ref name=evo1 />). The adaptive significance, occurrence, and variability of male infanticide remains unclear in many species because there is still lack enough data to compare directly the lifetime reproductive success of the “infanticidal” versus alternative “noninfanticidal” strategies.<ref name=IFS /> Whatever the adaptive significance of infanticide maybe, the behavior and counterstrategies against it are clearly costly.<ref name=IFS /><ref name=counter /> Further, the implications of infanticide and cannibalism for populations that engage in group cannibalism such as chimpanzees could be far-reaching. <ref name=just21 /> In addition to addressing these points, future research into infanticide could reveal the genetic basis for the behavior for non-human primates and answer questions surrounding the curious existence of such a behavior in animals. | |||
==References== | ==References== |
Latest revision as of 01:40, 7 December 2019
Introduction
Infanticide (in animals) generally refers to the killing of an infant or a young offspring by an adult or mature individual of the same species and is observed in a variety of species ranging from humans to microscopic rotifers and especially in primates. Both males and females can be the perpetrators of infanticide in animals and both parents (filial infanticide) and non-parent individuals have been observed to display the behavior.[1][2][3] Filial infanticide, which can be accompanied by cannibalism (filial cannibalism), is widespread in fishes and is also seen in terrestrial animals. It has been estimated that infanticide occurs in 25% of all mammals and, in some of these populations, infanticide is a major contributor to infant mortality.[4]
Many primates such as the gorilla, chimpanzee, baboon, and langur have been known to practice infanticide while others, such as the orangutan, bonobo and mouse lemur have not been observed to do so.[4] Although previously considered pathological and maladaptive and attributed to environmental conditions such as overcrowding and captivity [5], there are currently several explanations for why infanticide has evolved in non-human primate communities. Interestingly, the frequency and distribution of infanticide seems to suggest that there is a correlation between infanticide risk and social organization in mammals and primates. [6][7] Male infanticide occurs most frequently in social species, less frequently in solitary species and least frequently in monogamous species.[7] The fitness advantage the behavior has, the associated costs and the different ways it is thought to have affected the structure of primate populations make the evolution and genetics of the behavior very interesting to study.
Section 1 Evolution and Genetics of the Behavior
Several explanations have been proposed for the existence and evolution of infanticide in non- human primates. The benefits of infanticide for male non-human primates, and its costs to females, probably vary across mammalian social and mating systems [7] and between different primate species as well. Infanticidal behavior can be adaptive because of nutritive benefits individuals gain from cannibalism, conservation of resources by eliminating competitors and paternal manipulation in which parents terminate investment in particular offspring by killing (and often consuming) them [8]. The prevailing adaptive explanation for the behavior, however, has been the sexual-selection hypothesis according to which males increase reproductive opportunity by killing unrelated, unweaned offspring, thus hastening the mother's next ovulation, at which time the infanticidal male can mate with her [9]. Alternate nonadaptive explanations such as social-pathology and side effect hypotheses have also been proposed.[9]
Males in primate groups often exploit infants to which they are not related in ways that lead to the death of the infant.[8] Several primate species have been observed to engage in the killing and subsequent cannibalism of infants.[8] This type of behavior has also been seen in the great apes such as bonobos and chimpanzees.[10][3] In chimpanzees, cannibalism has been suggested to be the primary function of infanticide.[3] Infants may also be used as protective buffers in primate groups during agonistic encounters (conflicts) between males which can result in the death of the infant.[2] Females in primate groups also kill or cannibalize infants although usually for motivations different from nutrition.[1]
Maternal infanticide, although very rarely observed in non-human primates, has been considered as a way of paternal manipulation to end investment in certain offspring. [1] A study investigating maternal infanticide in tamarins has concluded that maternal infanticide can occur in a group with a poor capacity to raise offspring, multiple breeding females and estimated low survival probability for the infants that are killed because of injuries or apparent weakness.[1] The same study also found that the infants that were killed were cannibalized by their mothers. However, the study indicates that the mothers likely did not kill their offspring to exploit the meat.[1] The victim of infanticide for parental manipulation does not necessarily have to be defective but can simply be born at a bad time, consequently being a supplementary cost to the parents. [9]
On the other hand, the evolution of (nonparental) male infanticide has often been explained by the sexual selection hypothesis, which posits that infanticide improves male reproductive success by shortening the interbirth intervals of the mothers of the killed offspring.[9][11] Notably in primates, male infanticide can increase the reproductive success of male individuals by accelerating the return of females to fertilizable condition and ensuring that a greater percentages of the individuals’ offspring become the next generation when the male kills the offspring of other rival males [8]. The sexual selection hypothesis is supported by findings that indicate that infanticide mostly occurs in social species, less in solitary species, and least in monogamous species since according to the sexual selection hypothesis, infanticide would be most adaptive in stable bisexual groups where a few males monopolize reproduction over short periods of time [7].In social species where a few males monopolize reproduction over short periods, killing the offspring of males who had previously been monopolizing reproduction would be adaptive for the individuals committing infanticide. In monogamous species, since pairs of reproducing adults live together with their offspring their whole lives, male infanticide of unrelated infants would not be as adaptive for individuals. A study into infanticide in langur monkeys has also shown that male attackers were not related to their infant victim and were likely the fathers of subsequent infants based on DNA analyses, further supporting the hypothesis.[12]
While the majority of the explanations for why infanticide exists as a behavior posit that infanticide is an adaptive heritable behavior in primates, some have explained infanticide as a pathological behavior that is non-adaptive and does not need an evolutionary explanation.[8][5] One such hypothesis, social pathology, argues that infanticide is genetically inconsequential and maladaptive in normal conditions.[8] The hypothesis states that infanticide results from certain conditions created by human-induced disturbances or changes in nature.[8] Social disturbances such as overcrowding have been documented to have resulted in infanticide in rats [13], and similar factors may be at play in primate communities in which infanticide has been observed. Though the social pathology hypothesis may apply to some cases of infanticide [14], increasing evidence has pointed to the sexual selection hypothesis for the most likely explanation of the prevalence of infanticide in primate communities.[15]
One such evidence supporting the sexual selection hypothesis comes from a study conducted on house mice showing that infanticide is a highly liable and heritable behavior.[16] The study also concludes that there is a sex difference in the genetic substrate that regulates the inheritance of infanticidal behavior.[16] Recent advances in technology have shown that genetics heavily influences behavior or behavioral phenotype in primates and have led to the beginning of the identification of genes mediating different behaviors [17]. Though it is not clear which genes are involved in primate infanticide, there is likely a genetic basis for the behavior that is yet to be discovered.
Variation in social organization and mating systems may have favored or prevented the evolution of infanticide by males in mammalian species. The distribution of male infanticide is closely associated with the absence of seasonal or annual breeding. Seasonal breeders cannot resume (reproductive) cycling before the next breeding season. [7] As a result, infanticide in seasonal breeders doesn't accelerate the mother's return to sexual activity, relatively decreasing its adaptive advantage.[7] Furthermore, infanticide is more likely to occur in mammalian species where there are more males per females in social groups and dominant males obtain a higher share of reproduction in a given season but maintain their dominant position for short periods of time.[7] In these species, new dominant males could kill the infants of the previous dominant male so that more of the new dominant male’s genes are transferred to the next generation.[7]
Section 2 Costs of Infanticide and Counter-startegies
Whatever the benefit of male infanticide to the perpetrators, there is little doubt that it usually imposes a substantive cost on females’ fitness and reproductive success (as well as infants), particularly in the slowly reproducing species in which infanticide due to sexual selection is expected to operate.[8] This is thought to have led to the evolution of several female counter-strategies against infanticide ranging from physical defense of infants from infanticidal individuals to promiscuous mating to confuse paternity.[8] In addition to these counter-strategies, males are also thought to display behavior that prevents infanticide when their own offspring is involved.[18]
In many primate species, mothers have been documented to increase visual monitoring of potentially infanticidal male and/or offspring, and restrain and/or maintain proximity to infants more under conditions of heightened infanticide risk.[8] Males may also physically defend their own offspring.[18] In social primates such as baboons and macaques, the main role of male-male aggression may be to provide exclusive reproductive access to females and defend them and their young from stranger males.[18] Both of these counter-strategies result in costs such as expenditure of extra energy and increased risk of injuries. [18]
Another female counter-strategy against male infanticide (carried out due to reasons outlined by the sexual selection hypothesis) that has thought to have evolved in primates is promiscuity or multi-male mating.[8] Promiscuous mating as a result of some reproductive adaptations/changes creates paternity confusion among the primate group thereby decreasing the likelihood of infanticide since perpetrators would not be sure if they were killing their children.[18] Among primates, 62% of species with female susceptibility to infanticide show promiscuity, compared to the 11% of non-vulnerable species showing promiscuity. [19] This suggests that there is some correlation between infanticide and promiscuity in primates. Females in some primate species such as langur and gelada have also been known to terminate pregnancies to end investment in that offspring early after exposure to new replacement males through a process known as the Bruce effect.[8] Other female reproductive counter-strategies include but are not limited to concealed ovulation (paternity is obscured by the absence of reliable signals of ovulation and/or by variability in the timing of ovulation) and copulation with specific males (like those most likely to commit infanticide or more likely to protect the offspring). [8] A large set of potential female counterstrategies involves deterrence through cooperation with conspecifics (individuals of the same species). [8] Females in some primate species may form associations or coalitions with other females and with males to defend their offspring and reduce the risk of infanticide. [8]
Both infanticide and the counter-strategies against can have their own costs. Increased risk of injuries for both the perpetrator and the defender is one cost. [18] Some of the counter strategies listed above such as multi-male mating or physical defense are generally associated with the increased expenditure of energy, increased exposure to predation and, obviously, increased risk of injury for both the perpetrator and defender.[18] Counter strategies like multi-male mating and female mate choice could limit choice for good genes since the females would be forced to mate with specific males, [18] possibly affecting populations as a whole.
Section 3 Effects on Social Organization of Primates
Male infanticide has been proposed to alter social evolution in mammals (and primates) by favoring female gregariousness or permanent male-female associations or by promoting a higher number of resident males that may help mothers to protect their infants from male intruders.[7] It has also been proposed that male infanticide likely led to social monogamy in primates and, potentially, humans.[6] However, these theories have been criticized[7][20] and some have pointed out that these theories are not based on enough data, but make assumptions that are likely incorrect.[20] It is also important to consider that infanticide risk is low in solitary species, the ancestral mammalian social organization, and is unlikely to motivate evolutionary transitions from a solitary lifestyle to other social organizations.[7] Phylogenetic reconstructions have also shown that male infanticide is significantly more likely to evolve after transitions toward group living than in their solitary ancestors. [7] As mentioned above, male infanticide could have led to the evolution of multi-male mating or promiscuity of females in some primates species. [7][8] In support of this hypothesis, phylogenetic reconstructions indicate that relative testis size (which is an indicator of sperm competition) is larger in lineages in which more time has elapsed since the evolution of infanticide. [7] In addition, infanticide is lost in lineages with large testes, indicating that infanticide may disappear where female attempts to prevent male sexual monopolization are successful. [7]
Section 4 Microbiome
According to the resource competition hypothesis animals engage in infanticide to exploit the infants in different ways. [8] Adults of primate population often cannibalize the infant they kill, thereby gaining nutritive benefits from the action. [3][10] Since cannibalism can increase the risk of pathogen and disease transmission in animals [21], infanticide followed by cannibalism may decrease the fitness of individuals in primate species engaging in this behavior. Although cannibalism may be an unlikely disease transmission pathway in many primate species because the epidemiological conditions necessary for its spread are rarely met in natural populations, it is suspected that such transmission occurs in animals such as chimpanzees where cannibalism occurs by socially organized groups of males (group cannibalism). [22] Therefore, it is thought that group cannibalism including such cannibalism that follows infanticide (which has been documented in chimpanzees)[3][10], may play an important role in the transmission and maintenance of blood-borne infections like simian immunodeficiency virus or different types of hepatitis in these populations. [22]
Conclusion
The adaptive significance of infanticide, if the behavior is considered adaptive for a population, most likely differs from population to population meaning the advantage of the strategy is multifarious, not unitary. [8] It is important to consider various factors that could be affecting the frequency and distribution of infanticide (such as overcrowding in a population[8]) or those that could be affected by it (such as testes size[7]). The adaptive significance, occurrence, and variability of male infanticide remains unclear in many species because there is still lack enough data to compare directly the lifetime reproductive success of the “infanticidal” versus alternative “noninfanticidal” strategies.[8] Whatever the adaptive significance of infanticide maybe, the behavior and counterstrategies against it are clearly costly.[8][18] Further, the implications of infanticide and cannibalism for populations that engage in group cannibalism such as chimpanzees could be far-reaching. [22] In addition to addressing these points, future research into infanticide could reveal the genetic basis for the behavior for non-human primates and answer questions surrounding the curious existence of such a behavior in animals.
References
- ↑ 1.0 1.1 1.2 1.3 1.4 Culot, Laurence, et al. “Reproductive Failure, Possible Maternal Infanticide, and Cannibalism in Wild Moustached Tamarins, Saguinus Mystax.” Primates, vol. 52, no. 2, 2011, pp. 179–186., doi:10.1007/s10329-011-0238-6.
- ↑ 2.0 2.1 Dunbar, R. I. M. “Infant-Use by Male Gelada in Agonistic Contexts: Agonistic Buffering, Progeny Protection or Soliciting Support?” Primates, vol. 25, no. 1, 1984, pp. 28–35., doi:10.1007/bf02382293
- ↑ 3.0 3.1 3.2 3.3 3.4 Hamai, Miya, et al. “New Records of within-Group Infanticide and Cannibalism in Wild Chimpanzees.” Primates, vol. 33, no. 2, 1992, pp. 151–162., doi:10.1007/bf02382746.
- ↑ 4.0 4.1 [Dunham, Will. “Infanticide Common among Adult Males in Many Mammal Species.” Reuters, Thomson Reuters, 13 Nov. 2014, https://www.reuters.com/article/us-science-infanticide/infanticide-common-among-adult-males-in-many-mammal-species-idUSKCN0IX2BA20141113.]
- ↑ 5.0 5.1 Moore, Jim. “Population Density, Social Pathology, and Behavioral Ecology.” Primates, vol. 40, no. 1, 1999, pp. 1–22., doi:10.1007/bf02557698.
- ↑ 6.0 6.1 Opie, C., et al. “Male Infanticide Leads to Social Monogamy in Primates.” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, vol. 110, no. 33, 2013, pp. 13328–13332., doi:10.1073/pnas.1307903110
- ↑ 7.00 7.01 7.02 7.03 7.04 7.05 7.06 7.07 7.08 7.09 7.10 7.11 7.12 7.13 7.14 7.15 Lukas, D., and E. Huchard. “The Evolution of Infanticide by Males in Mammalian Societies.” Science, vol. 346, no. 6211, 2014, pp. 841–844., doi:10.1126/science.1257226.
- ↑ 8.00 8.01 8.02 8.03 8.04 8.05 8.06 8.07 8.08 8.09 8.10 8.11 8.12 8.13 8.14 8.15 8.16 8.17 8.18 8.19 8.20 Palombit, Ryne A. “Infanticide as Sexual Conflict: Coevolution of Male Strategies and Female Counterstrategies.” Cold Spring Harbor Perspectives in Biology, vol. 7, no. 6, 2015, doi:10.1101/cshperspect.a017640
- ↑ 9.0 9.1 9.2 9.3 Hrdy, Sarah Blaffer. “Infanticide among Animals: A Review, Classification, and Examination of the Implications for the Reproductive Strategies of Females.” Ethology and Sociobiology, vol. 1, no. 1, 1979, pp. 13–40., doi:10.1016/0162-3095(79)90004-9
- ↑ 10.0 10.1 10.2 Callaway, Ewen. “Hippy Apes Caught Cannibalising Their Young.” New Scientist, 1 Feb. 2010, https://www.newscientist.com/article/dn18454-hippy-apes-caught-cannibalising-their-young/
- ↑ Harano, Tomohiro, and Nobuyuki Kutsukake. “The Evolution of Male Infanticide in Relation to Sexual Selection in Mammalian Carnivores.” Evolutionary Ecology, vol. 32, no. 1, 2017, pp. 1–8., doi:10.1007/s10682-017-9925-0
- ↑ Borries, Carola, et al. “DNA Analyses Support the Hypothesis That Infanticide Is Adaptive in Langur Monkeys.” Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, vol. 266, no. 1422, July 1999, pp. 901–904., doi:10.1098/rspb.1999.0721.
- ↑ Ramsden, Edmund. “The Urban Aminal: Population Density and Social Pathology in Rodent and Humans.” Bulletin of the World Health Organization, vol. 87, no. 2, Jan. 2009, pp. 82–82., doi:10.2471/blt.09.062836
- ↑ Feh, C., and B. Munkhtuya. “Male Infanticide and Paternity Analyses in a Socially Natural Herd of Przewalskis Horses: Sexual Selection?” Behavioral Processes, vol. 78, no. 3, 2008, pp. 335–339., doi:10.1016/j.beproc.2007.12.009.
- ↑ Janson, Charles H., and Carel P. Van Schaik. “The Behavioral Ecology of Infanticide by Males.” Infanticide by Males and Its Implications, Feb. 2000, pp. 469–494., doi:10.1017/cbo9780511542312.021.
- ↑ 16.0 16.1 <Perrigo, Glenn, et al. “Genetic Mediation of Infanticide and Parental Behavior in Male and Female Domestic and Wild Stock House Mice.” Behavior Genetics, vol. 23, no. 6, 1993, pp. 525–531., doi:10.1007/bf01068143
- ↑ Rogers, Jeffrey. “The Behavioral Genetics of Nonhuman Primates: Status and Prospects.” American Journal of Physical Anthropology, vol. 165, 2018, pp. 23–36., doi:10.1002/ajpa.23384.
- ↑ 18.0 18.1 18.2 18.3 18.4 18.5 18.6 18.7 18.8 Agrell, Jep, et al. “Counter-Strategies to Infanticide in Mammals: Costs and Consequences.” Oikos, vol. 83, no. 3, 1998, p. 507., doi:10.2307/3546678.
- ↑ Noordwijk, Maria A. Van, and Carel P. Van Schaik. “Reproductive Patterns in Eutherian Mammals: Adaptations against Infanticide?” Infanticide by Males and Its Implications, Feb. 2000, pp. 322–360., doi:10.1017/cbo9780511542312.016
- ↑ 20.0 20.1 Dixson, A. F. “Male Infanticide and Primate Monogamy.” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, vol. 110, no. 51, May 2013, doi:10.1073/pnas.1318645110.
- ↑ Trapanese, Cinzia, et al. “Prolonged Care and Cannibalism of Infant Corpse by Relatives in Semi-Free-Ranging Capuchin Monkeys.” Primates, Mar. 2019, doi:10.1007/s10329-019-00747-8.
- ↑ 22.0 22.1 22.2 Rudolf, Volker H.w, and Janis Antonovics. “Disease Transmission by Cannibalism: Rare Event or Common Occurrence?” Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, vol. 274, no. 1614, 2007, pp. 1205–1210., doi:10.1098/rspb.2006.0449
Edited by MEHERET OURGESSA, student of Joan Slonczewski for BIOL 116 Information in Living Systems, 2019, Kenyon College.