Infanticide in Primates

From MicrobeWiki, the student-edited microbiology resource

Introduction

Infanticide (in animals) generally refers to the killing of an infant or a young offspring by an adult or mature individual of the same species and is observed in a variety of species ranging from humans to microscopic rotifers and especially in primates. Both males and females can be the perpetrators of infanticide in animals and both parents (filial infanticide) and non-parent individuals have been observed to display the behavior. Filial infanticide, which can be accompanied by cannibalism (filial cannibalism), is widespread in fishes and is also seen in terrestrial animals. It has been estimated that infanticide occurs in 25% of all mammals and, in some of these populations, infanticide is a major contributor to infant mortality.[1]


Many primates such as the gorilla, chimpanzee, baboon, and langur have been known to practice infanticide while others, such as the orangutan, bonobo and mouse lemur have not been observed to do so[1]. Although previously considered pathological and maladaptive and attributed to environmental conditions such as overcrowding and captivity [2], there are currently several explanations for why infanticide has evolved in non-human primate communities. Interestingly, the frequency and distribution of infanticide seems to suggest that there is a correlation between infanticide risk and social organization in mammals and primates. Male infanticide occurs most frequently in social species, less frequently in solitary species and least frequently in monogamous species.[3] The fitness advantage the behavior has, the associated costs and the different ways it is thought to have affected the structure of primate populations have made the evolution and genetics of the behavior very interesting to study.

Section 1 Evolution and Genetics of Behavior

Several explanations have been proposed for the existence and evolution of infanticide in non- human primates. The benefits of infanticide for male non-human primates, and its costs to females, probably vary across mammalian social and mating systems [3] and between different primate species as well. Infanticidal behavior can be adaptive because of nutritive benefits individuals gain from cannibalism, conservation of resources by eliminating competitors and paternal manipulation in which parents terminate investment in particular offspring by killing (and often consuming) them [4]. The prevailing adaptive explanation for the behavior, however, has been the sexual-selection hypothesis according to which males increase reproductive opportunity by killing unrelated, unweaned offspring, thus hastening the mother's next ovulation, at which time the infanticidal male can mate with her [5]. Alternate nonadaptive explanations such as social-pathology and side effect hypotheses have also been proposed.[5]

Males in primate groups often exploit infants to which they are not related in ways that lead to the death of the infant.[4] Several primate species have been observed to engage in the killing and subsequent cannibalism of infants.[4] This type of behavior has also been seen in the great apes such as bonobos and chimpanzees.[6][7] In chimpanzees, cannibalism has been suggested to be the primary function of infanticide.[7] Infants may also be used as protective buffers in primate groups during agnostic encounters (conflicts) between males which can result in the death of the infant.[8] Females in primate groups also kill or cannibalize infants although usually for motivations different from nutrition.[9]

Maternal infanticide, although very rarely observed in non-human primates, has been considered as a way of paternal manipulation to end investment in certain offspring. [9] A study investigating maternal infanticide in tamarins has concluded that maternal infanticide can occur in a group with a poor capacity to raise offspring, multiple breeding females and estimated low survival probability for the infants that are killed because of injuries or apparent weakness.[9] The same study also found that the infants that were killed were cannibalized by their mothers. However, the study indicates that the mothers likely did not kill their offspring to exploit the meat.[9] The victim of infanticide for parental manipulation does not necessarily have to be defective but can simply be born at a bad time, consequently being a supplementary cost to the parents. [5]

Baby gelada baboons play in the Simien Mountains of Ethiopia in this Nov. 30, 2012, picture.(AP Photo)
https://www.newser.com/story/198646/why-25-of-mammals-commit-infanticide.html

On the other hand, the evolution of (nonparental) male infanticide has often been explained by the sexual selection hypothesis, which posits that infanticide improves male reproductive success by shortening the interbirth intervals of the mothers of the killed offspring.[5][10] Notably in primates, male infanticide can increase the reproductive success of male individuals by accelerating the return of females to fertilizable condition and ensuring that a greater percentages of the individuals’ offspring become the next generation when the male kills the offspring of other rival males [4]. The sexual selection hypothesis is supported by findings that indicate that infanticide mostly occurs in social species, less in solitary species, and least in monogamous species since according to the sexual selection hypothesis, infanticide would be most adaptive in stable bisexual groups where a few males monopolize reproduction over short periods of time [3].In social species where a few males monopolize reproduction over short periods, killing the offspring of males who had previously been monopolizing reproduction would be adaptive for the individuals committing infanticide. In monogamous species, since pairs of reproducing adults live together with their offspring their whole lives, male infanticide of unrelated infants would not be as adaptive for individuals. A study into infanticide in langur monkeys has also shown that male attackers were not related to their infant victim and were likely the fathers of subsequent infants based on DNA analyses, further supporting the hypothesis.[11]


While the majority of the explanations for why infanticide exists as a behavior posit that infanticide is an adaptive heritable behavior in primates, some have explained infanticide as a pathological behavior that is non-adaptive and does not need an evolutionary explanation.[4][2] One such hypothesis, social pathology, argues that infanticide is genetically inconsequential and maladaptive in normal conditions.[4] The hypothesis states that infanticide results from certain conditions created by human-induced disturbances or changes in nature.[4] Social disturbances such as overcrowding have been documented to have resulted in infanticide in rats [12], and similar factors may be at play in primate communities in which infanticide has been observed. Though the social pathology hypothesis may apply to some cases of infanticide [13], increasing evidence has pointed to the sexual selection hypothesis for the most likely explanation of the prevalence of infanticide in primate communities.[14]

Male infanticide has been reported for about half of all species this sample (open circles) and seems to have evolved independently multiple times. It most frequently occurs in social species (dark gray branches), less frequently in solitary species (light gray branches), and least frequently in monogamous species (black branches). Animal drawings are from phylopic.org.
https://science.sciencemag.org/content/346/6211/841

One such evidence supporting the sexual selection hypothesis comes from a study conducted on house mice showing that infanticide is a highly liable and heritable behavior.[15] The study also concludes that there is a sex difference in the genetic substrate that regulates the inheritance of infanticidal behavior [15]. Recent advances in technology have shown that genetics heavily influences behavior or behavioral phenotype in primates and have led to the beginning of the identification of genes mediating different behaviors [16]. Though it is not clear which genes are involved in primate infanticide, there is likely a genetic basis for the behavior that is yet to be discovered.

Variation in social organization and mating systems may have favored or prevented the evolution of infanticide by males in mammalian species. The distribution of male infanticide is closely associated with the absence of seasonal or annual breeding. Seasonal breeders cannot resume (reproductive) cycling before the next breeding season. [3] As a result, infanticide in seasonal breeders doesn't accelerate the mother's return to sexual activity, relatively decreasing its adaptive advantage.[3] Furthermore, infanticide is more likely to occur in mammalian species where there are more males per females in social groups and dominant males obtain a higher share of reproduction in a given season but maintain their dominant position for short periods of time.[3] In these species, new dominant males could kill the infants of the previous dominant male so that more of the new dominant male’s genes are transferred to the next generation.[3]

Section 2 Costs of Infanticide and Counter-startegies

Whatever the benefit of male infanticide to the perpetrators, there is little doubt that it usually imposes a substantive cost on females’ fitness and reproductive success (as well as infants), particularly in the slowly reproducing species in which infanticide due to sexual selection is expected to operate.[4] This is thought to have led to the evolution of several female counter-strategies against infanticide ranging from physical defense of infants from infanticidal individuals to promiscuous mating to confuse paternity.[4] In addition to these counter-strategies, males are also thought to display behavior that prevents infanticide when their own offspring is involved.[17]

In many primate species, mothers have been documented to increase visual monitoring of potentially infanticidal male and/or offspring, and restrain and/or maintain proximity to infants more under conditions of heightened infanticide risk.[4] Males may also physically defend their own offspring.[17] In social primates such as baboons and macaques, the main role of male-male aggression may be to provide exclusive reproductive access to females and defend them and their young from stranger males.[17] Both of these counter-strategies result in costs such as expenditure of extra energy and increased risk of injuries. [17]

Section 3 Microbiome

Include some current research, with a second image.

Conclusion

Overall text length should be at least 1,000 words (before counting references), with at least 2 images. Include at least 5 references under Reference section.


References

  1. 1.0 1.1 [Dunham, Will. “Infanticide Common among Adult Males in Many Mammal Species.” Reuters, Thomson Reuters, 13 Nov. 2014, https://www.reuters.com/article/us-science-infanticide/infanticide-common-among-adult-males-in-many-mammal-species-idUSKCN0IX2BA20141113.]
  2. 2.0 2.1 Moore, Jim. “Population Density, Social Pathology, and Behavioral Ecology.” Primates, vol. 40, no. 1, 1999, pp. 1–22., doi:10.1007/bf02557698.
  3. 3.0 3.1 3.2 3.3 3.4 3.5 3.6 Lukas, D., and E. Huchard. “The Evolution of Infanticide by Males in Mammalian Societies.” Science, vol. 346, no. 6211, 2014, pp. 841–844., doi:10.1126/science.1257226.
  4. 4.0 4.1 4.2 4.3 4.4 4.5 4.6 4.7 4.8 4.9 Palombit, Ryne A. “Infanticide as Sexual Conflict: Coevolution of Male Strategies and Female Counterstrategies.” Cold Spring Harbor Perspectives in Biology, vol. 7, no. 6, 2015, doi:10.1101/cshperspect.a017640
  5. 5.0 5.1 5.2 5.3 Hrdy, Sarah Blaffer. “Infanticide among Animals: A Review, Classification, and Examination of the Implications for the Reproductive Strategies of Females.” Ethology and Sociobiology, vol. 1, no. 1, 1979, pp. 13–40., doi:10.1016/0162-3095(79)90004-9
  6. Callaway, Ewen. “Hippy Apes Caught Cannibalising Their Young.” New Scientist, 1 Feb. 2010, https://www.newscientist.com/article/dn18454-hippy-apes-caught-cannibalising-their-young/
  7. 7.0 7.1 Hamai, Miya, et al. “New Records of within-Group Infanticide and Cannibalism in Wild Chimpanzees.” Primates, vol. 33, no. 2, 1992, pp. 151–162., doi:10.1007/bf02382746.
  8. Dunbar, R. I. M. “Infant-Use by Male Gelada in Agonistic Contexts: Agonistic Buffering, Progeny Protection or Soliciting Support?” Primates, vol. 25, no. 1, 1984, pp. 28–35., doi:10.1007/bf02382293
  9. 9.0 9.1 9.2 9.3 Culot, Laurence, et al. “Reproductive Failure, Possible Maternal Infanticide, and Cannibalism in Wild Moustached Tamarins, Saguinus Mystax.” Primates, vol. 52, no. 2, 2011, pp. 179–186., doi:10.1007/s10329-011-0238-6.
  10. Harano, Tomohiro, and Nobuyuki Kutsukake. “The Evolution of Male Infanticide in Relation to Sexual Selection in Mammalian Carnivores.” Evolutionary Ecology, vol. 32, no. 1, 2017, pp. 1–8., doi:10.1007/s10682-017-9925-0
  11. Borries, Carola, et al. “DNA Analyses Support the Hypothesis That Infanticide Is Adaptive in Langur Monkeys.” Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, vol. 266, no. 1422, July 1999, pp. 901–904., doi:10.1098/rspb.1999.0721.
  12. Ramsden, Edmund. “The Urban Aminal: Population Density and Social Pathology in Rodent and Humans.” Bulletin of the World Health Organization, vol. 87, no. 2, Jan. 2009, pp. 82–82., doi:10.2471/blt.09.062836
  13. Feh, C., and B. Munkhtuya. “Male Infanticide and Paternity Analyses in a Socially Natural Herd of Przewalskis Horses: Sexual Selection?” Behavioral Processes, vol. 78, no. 3, 2008, pp. 335–339., doi:10.1016/j.beproc.2007.12.009.
  14. Janson, Charles H., and Carel P. Van Schaik. “The Behavioral Ecology of Infanticide by Males.” Infanticide by Males and Its Implications, Feb. 2000, pp. 469–494., doi:10.1017/cbo9780511542312.021.
  15. 15.0 15.1 <Perrigo, Glenn, et al. “Genetic Mediation of Infanticide and Parental Behavior in Male and Female Domestic and Wild Stock House Mice.” Behavior Genetics, vol. 23, no. 6, 1993, pp. 525–531., doi:10.1007/bf01068143
  16. Rogers, Jeffrey. “The Behavioral Genetics of Nonhuman Primates: Status and Prospects.” American Journal of Physical Anthropology, vol. 165, 2018, pp. 23–36., doi:10.1002/ajpa.23384.
  17. 17.0 17.1 17.2 17.3 Agrell, Jep, et al. “Counter-Strategies to Infanticide in Mammals: Costs and Consequences.” Oikos, vol. 83, no. 3, 1998, p. 507., doi:10.2307/3546678.


Edited by MEHERET OURGESSA, student of Joan Slonczewski for BIOL 116 Information in Living Systems, 2019, Kenyon College.